Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

The Effect of Removing Sentencing Credits on Inmate Misbehavior

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Journal of Quantitative Criminology Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Objective

To examine the effect of prison officials’ decisions to remove good time credits in response to prison rule violations on subsequent inmate misbehavior.

Methods

Data pertaining to all inmates admitted to prison in a Midwestern state during 2009 who committed a rule violation were examined using two different methods, a multi-level analysis of a longitudinal person-period dataset and a comparison of the prison misconduct rates for inmates who lost good time during their first year of confinement to those for a matched control group of inmates who did not lose good time.

Results

The multi-level longitudinal analysis revealed that inmates who lost good time in response to a prison rule violation were typically less likely to commit misconduct in the periods after they lost good time relative to periods before inmates lost good time, but the size of the observed effects were small. For the most part, the analyses of the matched sample of inmates who lost good time during their first year of confinement versus those inmates who did not lose good time revealed that losing good time did not affect inmates’ rates of subsequent misconduct.

Conclusions

Findings suggest that good time laws have little to no specific deterrent effects on inmate misbehavior.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Inmates can earn good time credits in 32 states, while 37 states have laws that afford inmates earned time credits; many states permit inmates to be awarded both types of sentencing credits (Lawrence and Lyons 2011).

  2. Disciplinary committee members recuse themselves from hearings pertaining to incidents in which they were a witness, reporting officer, or investigating officer.

  3. During the study period, prison officials removed good time in addition to imposing segregation or cell isolation in over 90% of the cases examined. In states with overcrowded prisons (and segregation units), such as the state under study here, it is common for prison officials to impose cell isolation in lieu of segregation.

  4. The study conducted by Drake et al. (2009) could be considered an assessment of the effects of earning sentencing credits on offender behavior, but the researchers were unable to determine if all of the offenders in their sample earned additional release time. On average, however, inmates in their sample earned 60 additional days of release time (E. Drake, personal communication, 3rd November, 2014).

  5. Inmates were removed from the sample if they had served their entire sentence in a local jail, were sentenced to life in prison, or were transferred to/from another jurisdiction (N =153). Approximately 74% of the inmates admitted to prison in 2009 were convicted of a rule violation during the study period.

  6. Not all of the inmates with mental health problems are placed in a mental health unit during their confinement in this Midwestern state. Therefore, the measure of mental health problems does not include all inmates who suffered from mental health problems during the study period. Further, the measure does not assess the severity or recentness of an inmate’s problem. No other measures of inmates’ mental illness were available electronically from the Midwestern state’s Department of Corrections, however.

  7. An omitted variable could be prior sanctions (other than losing good time). We assessed the possibility of including several different measures or prior sanctions; however, each of these measures was highly collinear with prior violation history. We retained prior violation history because its’ correlation with both losing good time and subsequent misconduct was stronger than the prior sanctions measures.

  8. As noted above, the second analysis was limited to inmates convicted of a rule violation during their first year of imprisonment in order to allow for an adequate follow-up period to assess whether losing good time had an effect on inmates’ subsequent misbehavior—most individuals (71 percent) admitted to prison in the Midwestern state during 2009 served less than two years in prison. Further, the majority of inmates who committed a rule violation (91 percent) during their imprisonment did so during their first year, and the majority of inmates who lost good time (70 percent) lost good time in response to a violation during their first year of imprisonment.

  9. Since good time is typically removed in addition to another sanction (e.g., segregation), it is possible that variation in the other sanction between the treatment and control group violated the stable unit treatment value assumption. We examined this possibility and found no significant differences in the other sanction received for those in treatment group versus those in the control group, assuming segregation and room restriction are treated the same. As noted, the prison system in the Midwestern state was severely overcrowded during the time of the study, and room restriction was frequently used by prison administrators in lieu of segregation due to the limited amount of segregation space (i.e., available segregation space in a prison is based on its design capacity).

  10. We used the formula provided by Hanushek and Jackson (1977) to transform the coefficient estimates into predicted probabilities. The other predictors in the model were held constant at their means.

  11. The category 15 days included one seven-day sanction and one 14-day sanction. The category 90 days included one 60-day sanction and one 75-day sanction.

  12. The distributions of the incidence of violent misconducts and the incidence of Class I misconducts were restricted to vary from 0-6 and 0-8, respectively, whereas the distribution of the incidence of misconducts was restricted to vary from 0-42. We restricted the distributions of these measures in order to capture more meaningful variation in these outcomes; less than 2% of the sample committed more than six violent misconducts or eight Class I misconducts, and less than 5% of sample committed more than 42 misconducts.

  13. The distributions of the incidence rate outcomes reported in Table 6 are skewed, and so significance tests were performed after transforming the distributions of the original limited counts of these events to negative binomial distributions and including an offset variable (time at risk) in the models.

References

  • Babcock W (1981) Due process in prison disciplinary proceedings. Boston College Law Review 22:1009–1096

    Google Scholar 

  • Bales WD, Miller CH (2012) The impact of determinate sentencing on prisoner misconduct. Journal of Criminal Justice 40:394–403

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Demleitner NV (2009) Good conduct time: How much and for whom? The unprincipled approach of the model penal code. Florida Law Review 61:777–796

    Google Scholar 

  • Drake E, Barnoski R, Aos S (2009) Increased earned release from prison: Impacts of a 2003 law on recidivism and crime costs.Revised. Washington State Institute for Public Policy, Olympia, WA

    Google Scholar 

  • Durlauf SN, Nagin DS (2011) Imprisonment and crime. Criminology & Public Policy 10(1):13–54

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Emshoff JG, Davidson WS (1987) The effect of “good time” credit on inmate behavior: A quasi-experiment. Criminal Justice and Behavior 14:335–351

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Flanagan T (1982) Discretion in the prison justice system: A study of sentencing on institutional disciplinary proceedings. Journal of Research in Crime and Delinquency 19:16–237

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Green DP, Winik D (2010) Using random judge assignments to estimate the effects of incarceration and probation on recidivism among drug offenders. Criminology 48(2):357–387

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Guo S, Fraser MW (2010) Propensity Score Analysis: Statistical Methods and Applications. Sage Publications, Thousand Oaks, CA

    Google Scholar 

  • Haney C (2003) Mental health issues in long-term solitary and “supermax” confinement. Crime and Delinquency 49:124–156

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hanushek E, Jackson J (1977) Statistical Methods for Social Scientists. Academic Press Inc, New York, NY

    Google Scholar 

  • Heimer K, Matsueda RL (1994) Role-taking, role commitment, and delinquency: A theory of differential social control. American Sociological Review 59:365–390

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Henrichson C, Delaney R (2012) The price of prisons: What incarceration costs taxpayers. Vera Institute of Justice, New York, NY

    Google Scholar 

  • Jacobs JB (1982) Sentencing by prison personnel: Good time. UCLA Law Review 30:217–270

    Google Scholar 

  • King K, Steiner B, Breach SR (2008) Violence in the supermax: A self-fulfilling prophecy. The Prison Journal 88:144–168

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kupers T (2008) What to do with the survivors? Coping with the long-term effects of isolated confinement. Criminal Justice and Behavior 35(8):1005–1016

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lawrence A (2009) Cutting corrections costs: Earned time policies for state prisoners. National Conference of State Legislatures, Washington, DC

    Google Scholar 

  • Lawrence A, Lyons D (2011) Principles of effective state sentencing and corrections policy: A report of the NCSL sentencing and corrections work group. National Conference of State Legislatures, Washington, DC

    Google Scholar 

  • Lemert EM (1951) Social Pathology. McGraw-Hill, New York, NY

    Google Scholar 

  • Lemert E (1967) Human Deviance, Social Problems and Social Control. Prentice Hall, Engelwood Cliffs, New Jersey

    Google Scholar 

  • Marx GT (1981) Ironies of social control: Authorities as contributors to deviance through escalation, nonenforcement and covert facilitation. Social Problems 28(3):221–246

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Memory JM, Guo G, Parker K, Sutton T (1999) Comparing disciplinary infraction rates of North Carolina fair sentencing and structured sentencing inmates: A natural experiment. The Prison Journal 79:45–71

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mitchell O, Cochran JC, Mears DP, Bales WD (2017) The effectiveness of prison for reducing drug offender recidivism: A regression discontinuity analysis. Journal of Experimental Criminology 13(1):1–27

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nagin DS, Paternoster R (1993) Enduring individual differences and rational choice theories of crime. Law and Society Review 27(3):467–496

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Osgood DW (2010) Statistical models of life events and criminal behavior. In: Piquero A, Weisburd D (eds) Handbook of quantitative criminology. Springer Publishing, New York, NY, pp 375–398

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Paternoster R, Iovanni L (1989) The labeling perspective and delinquency: An elaboration of the theory and an assessment of the evidence. Justice Quarterly 6(3):359–394

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pelka, M., Weckerly, E., Bonilla, C., & Wilson, E. (2014). Nebraska Justice Reinvestment A presentation to the Nebraska Justice Reinvestment Working Group. Lincoln, NE: October.

  • Raudenbush S, Bryk A (2002) Hierarchical Linear Models: Applications and Data Analysis Methods, 2nd edn. Sage Publications, Thousand Oaks, CA

    Google Scholar 

  • Riveland C (1999) Supermax Prisons: Overview and General Considerations. U.S. Department of Justice, Washington, DC

    Google Scholar 

  • Rosenbaum PR, Rubin DR (1985) Constructing a control group using multivariate matched sampling methods that incorporate the propensity score. American Statistician 39(1):33–38

    Google Scholar 

  • Royster v. McGinnis, 1973. 410 U.S. 263.

  • Sandin v. Conner, 1995. 515 U.S. 472.

  • Schriro D (2009) Is good time a good idea? A practitioner’s perspective. Federal Sentencing Reporter 21(3):179–181

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Smith PS (2006) The effects of solitary confinement on prison inmates: A brief history and review of the literature. In: Tonry M (ed) Crime and Justice: A Review of Research, vol 34. University of Chicago Press, Chicago, IL, pp 441–528

    Google Scholar 

  • Steiner B, Wooldredge J (2014) Comparing self-report to official measures of inmate misconduct. Justice Quarterly 31(6):1074–1101

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Taxman FS, Soule D, Gelb A (1999) Graduated sanctions: Stepping into accountable systems and offenders. The Prison Journal 79(2):182–204

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thoemmes, F. (2012). Propensity score matching in SPSS. arXiv preprint arXiv:1201.6385.

  • Toch H, Adams K, Grant JD (1989) Coping: Maladaptation in Prisons. Transaction Publishers, New Brunswick, NJ

    Google Scholar 

  • VanderWeele T, Arah OA (2011) Unmeasured confounding for general outcomes, treatments and confounders: Bias formulas for sensitivity analysis. Epidemiology 22(1):42–52

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Weisburd D, Chayet E (1989) Good time: An agenda for research. Criminal Justice and Behavior 16:183–195

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wilkins L (1964) Social Deviance. Tavistock, London, UK

    Google Scholar 

  • Wolff v. McDonnell, 1974. 418 U.S. 539.

Download references

Acknowledgements

This project was supported by grants from the Nebraska Center for Justice Research, a research unit of the School of Criminology and Criminal Justice at the University of Nebraska, Omaha and the Office of Research and Creative Activity at the University of Nebraska, Omaha. The opinions, findings, and conclusions or recommendations expressed in the publication are those of the authors and do not necessarily reflect the views of the University of Nebraska, Omaha. The authors wish to thank Dr. Abby Vandenberg and Dr. Robert Lytle for their assistance with the collection of the data for this study.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Benjamin Steiner.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Steiner, B., Cain, C.M. The Effect of Removing Sentencing Credits on Inmate Misbehavior. J Quant Criminol 35, 89–108 (2019). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10940-017-9372-7

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10940-017-9372-7

Keywords

Navigation